Responsive image
博碩士論文 etd-0628101-145816 詳細資訊
Title page for etd-0628101-145816
論文名稱
Title
台灣獼猴(Macaca cyclopis)聲音類型之研究
Vocal patterns in wild Formosan macaques (Macaca cyclopis)
系所名稱
Department
畢業學年期
Year, semester
語文別
Language
學位類別
Degree
頁數
Number of pages
94
研究生
Author
指導教授
Advisor
召集委員
Convenor
口試委員
Advisory Committee
口試日期
Date of Exam
2001-06-20
繳交日期
Date of Submission
2001-06-28
關鍵字
Keywords
台灣獼猴、聲音類型、壽山
Formosan macaques, vocal pattern
統計
Statistics
本論文已被瀏覽 5747 次,被下載 2140
The thesis/dissertation has been browsed 5747 times, has been downloaded 2140 times.
中文摘要
摘 要
本研究針對壽山地區的台灣獼猴進行調查,野外觀察時間自1999年10月至2000年10月,共記錄了375小時,其中包含55小時的掃描取樣(scan sampling)時間及320小時的焦點追蹤(focal animal sampling)時間。壽山地區台灣獼猴的叫聲至少有25種不同的聲音類型 (vocal pattern),其中包括32種不同的聲音 (vocal type)。在這25種聲音類型中,出現頻度最高的三種類型分別是coo calls (18.59%)、hack (17.78%) 及contact rattle (11.85%)。其餘的聲音類型還有squeak (11.34%)、noise and undulated scream (6.61%)、sneeze (4.54%)、greeting (3.77%)、squeal (3.41%)、vibrato growl (3.02%)、growl (2.86%)、cluck (2.54%)、female copulation call (2.45%)、squawk (2.14%)、tonal scream (2.03%)、alarm call (1.74%)、threat rattle (1.66%)、chuckle (0.92%)、rise (0.61%)、weeping (0.56%)、male copulation call (0.32%)、babble (0.30%)、whine (0.29%)、roar (0.27%)、bark (0.24%)及mounting grunt (0.15%)。
所有的聲音類型依其間有無中間型存在可分為二類:獨立訊號(Discrete signal)及層級訊號(Graded signal)。獨立訊號就是在各聲音類型間缺乏中間型聲音,包括coo call、greeting、contact rattle、mounting grunt、male and female copulation calls, babble、whine及weeping。而層級訊號則是在各聲音類型中有中間型態的聲音存在,包括了屬於敵對叫聲的threat rattle、growl、bark、roar、vibrato growl和alarm call, 還有chuckle及屬於屈服叫聲的noise and undulated scream、squeal、 tonal scream and squeak以及屬於難過不舒服(distress)的叫聲(包括squawk、hack、cluck和sneeze)。從發聲的情境來看,屬於接近聯繫的叫聲有coo calls, greeting and contact rattles,以減少或維持與其他成員之間的距離。Growls、threat rattles和vibrato growls 屬於敵對的叫聲,發生在群內成員間位序高者威脅位序低者或群對抗及威嚇其他物種時。屈服的叫聲包含noise and undulated scream, squeal, tonal scream及兩種squeak,其聲音結構有明顯變化。而交配叫聲及警戒聲則與特定的互動或特定的外在刺激有關。
特定社會因素、發聲者的動機及激動狀態可能會影響聲音結構。屈服叫聲的結構可能與敵對互動時,有否身體接觸有關。Noise and undulated scream 較常發生在有身體接觸的敵對互動;而squeal、tonal scream 及 tonal and compound squeak 較常出現在無身體接觸的敵對互動。
台灣獼猴之聲音行為在年齡間的差異比在性別間的差異更大。各年齡性別層的相對頻度及發聲率上,從嬰猴到成年猴遞降。在所有的聲音表現中各年齡性別層裡,嬰猴有最高的發聲率(每10小時有1142.2個聲音單位)及相對頻度(44.58%)。隨著年齡增加,視覺訊號不只補充了聲音訊號,也可能取代了聲音訊號。有四種聲音類型(coo call、greeting、squeak及hack)在各年齡性別層中的相對頻度是從嬰猴到成年猴遞降;而threat rattle及growl 這二種聲音類型的相對頻度則是從嬰猴到成年猴遞增。cluck、sneeze、whine、weeping及babble這五種聲音類型僅在幼猴及嬰猴出現。 而在squeak (64 %)、hack (63.6 %)、squawk (76%)、cluck (85.7 %)、sneeze (90 %)、weeping (88 %)及babble (100%)中,大多由嬰猴產生,而且其中有三種聲音類型主要是由年齡階層II的嬰猴發出。另外,只有嬰猴會發出babble;而roar及bark只由成年或亞成年猴產生。
在聲音類型的相對頻度上,成年猴間有較明顯的性別差異。除了roar外,大部分的聲音類型其在成年母猴的相對頻度及發聲率皆高於其在成年公猴的。另外,母猴在六種聲音類型中(vibrato growl、chuckle、contact rattle、squeal、squeak及hack)的相對頻度及發聲率皆高於同年齡層的公猴。此外,mounting grunt、male copulation call只出現在成年公猴;而female copulation call 則只出現在成年與亞成年母猴。
特定年齡性別層中發出或不發出某些聲音,可能與社會組織、生理形態特性及環境經驗有關。而台灣獼猴的聲音表現與其他同屬的獼猴,尤其是日本獼猴(M. fuscata)、恆河猴(M. mulatta)和綺帽獼猴(M. radiata)有高度相似性。這可能是由於親緣關係、棲地和社會組織相似的緣故。
Abstract
ABSTRACT
Field observation and of sound recording of Formosan macaques (Macaca cyclopis) at Mt. Longevity were conducted from Oct. 1999 to Oct. 2000. Spectrographic analyses revealed at least 25 basic patterns with 32 different vocal types from a total of 375 hrs recordings (55 hrs for scan sampling and 320 hrs for focal animal sampling). The three most frequent calls in overall vocal repertoire were coo calls (18.59%), hack (17.78%) and contact rattle (11.85%). The other calls included squeak (11.34%), noise and undulated scream (6.61%), sneeze (4.54%), greeting (3.77%), squeal (3.41%), vibrato growl (3.02%), growl (2.86%), cluck (2.54%), female copulation call (2.45%), squawk (2.14%), tonal scream (2.03%), alarm call (1.74%), threat rattle (1.66%), chuckle (0.92%), rise (0.61%), weeping (0.56%), male copulation call (0.32%), babble (0.30%), whine (0.29%), roar (0.27%), bark (0.24%) and mounting grunt (0.15%).
Formosan macaques employ a complex vocal system composed of discrete signals as well as graded signals which vocal patterns connected by intermediate gradations. Discrete signal included coo call, greeting, contact rattle, mounting grunt, two copulation calls, babble, whine, weeping. Whereas the graded signals included the aggressive signals (threat rattle, growl, bark, roar, vibrato growl and alarm call), chuckle, the submissive signals (noise and undulated scream, squeal, tonal scream and squeak) and the distress signals (squawk, hack, cluck and sneeze).
According to the context of emission, some of vocalizations could be divided into: (1) Affiliative contact calls: The calls, as the coo calls, greeting and contact rattles, may be summarized as affinitive contact calls for reduction and/or maintenance of close proximity between group members. (2) Aggressive calls: The growls, threat rattles and vibrato growls were uttered by dominant animals menacing sub-dominate group mates, members of other groups or other species. (3) Submissive calls: these submissive calls (including the various form of scream, squeal and squeak) with obviously structure differences. Male and female copulation calls and alarm call of M. cyclopis were clearly related to particular interactions or specific external stimulus.
The acoustic structures might be influenced by specific social factors, motivation or the arousal state of the callers. The acoustic structure of submissive calls was associated with the aggression with or without physical contact. Noise and undulated scream were usually used in aggressive interactions with physical contact (68%), whereas squeal, tonal scream and two types of squeak were used without physical contact (75%).
Age specific difference of vocal behavior seemed to more pronounced than asymmetries between the sexes. Infants have the highest vocal rate (1142.2 vocalizations/10 h) and relative frequency of vocalization (44.58 %) than other age/sex classes and they decreased from infants to adults. With increasing age, visual signals could not only complement vocal signals but also replace the vocal cues. The relative frequency of four vocal patterns (coo call, greeting, squeak and hack) decreased from infants to adults, whereas that of threat rattle and growl increased from infant to adults. On the other hand, there were five vocal patterns (including cluck, sneeze, whine, weeping and babble) only used by juveniles and infants. In addition, seven vocal patterns included squeak (64 %), hack (63.6 %), squawk (76%), cluck (85.7 %), sneeze (90 %), weeping (88 %) and babble (100%) mainly were used by infants. Among them, three vocal patterns (squeak, hack and squawk) were produced mostly by infant II. Only infants used babble. Roar and bark were only present in adults and sub-adults.
Obvious sex differences of relative frequencies of vocal patterns occurred in adults, the vocal activity of females consistently higher compared to that of males. In six patterns (vibrato growl, chuckle contact rattle, squeal, squeak and hack), females uttered more often than male peers. Mounting grunts and male copulation calls were exclusively used by adult males whereas female copulation calls were only produced by adult/sub-adult females.
Vocalizations were produced/ceased by certain sex/age class that might associate with social organization, morphological feature and circumstances experience. The vocal repertoires of Formosan macaques revealed the high similarities in the species within genus Macaca, especially to M. fuscata, M. mulatta and M. radiata. It may be due to phylogeny, habitats and social organizations.
目次 Table of Contents
TABLE OF CONTENTS
誌謝.......................................i
中文摘要..................................ii
Abstract..................................iv
Introduction...... .........................1
Materials and Methods...... ................2
Study site...............................2
Study groups and methods.................3
Terminology and analysis.................4
Results.. ...................................6
Part I. Vocal categories of Formosan macaques.........................6
Part II. Comparative analysis of age- and sex- specific patterns.....27
Discussion........ .........................29
Vocal patterns....... ................29
Social contexts......................31
Acoustic strutures...................33
Age- and sex- specific patterns......35
In response to potential predators...38
Male loud calls...... ................39
Summary...............................40
References..................................41
Tables......................................48
Figures.....................................64

參考文獻 References
References
AICH, H.; MOOS-HEILEN, R.; ZIMMERMANN, E. 1990. Vocalizations of adult gelada baboons (Theropithecus gelada): Acoustic structure and behavioural context. Folia Primatol., 55: 109-132.
AKMAJIAN, A.; DEMERS, R. A.; HARNISH, R. M. 1979. Linguistics: An Introduction to Language and Communication. Massachusetts: MIT Press, Cambridge, pp.31-51.
ALTMANN, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49: 227-267.
BAUERS, K. A. 1989. The role of vocal communication in the intra-group social dynamic s of stumptailed macaques (Macaca arctoides). PhD. Thesis, Univ. of Wisconsin, Madison.
BAUERS, K. A. 1993. A functional analaysis of staccato grunt vocalizations in the stumptailed macaque (Macaca arctoides). Ethology, 94: 147-161.
BAUERS, K. A.; DE WAAL, F. B. M. (1991). “Coo” vocalizations in stumptailed macaques: A controlled functional analysis. Behaviour, 119: 143-159.
BERTRANDE, M. 1969. The behavioral repertoire of the stumptail macaque. Biblthca primatol., Vol.11. Karger, Basel.
BROWN, H. C.; WASER, P. M. 1984. Hearing and communication in blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis). Anim. Behav., 32: 66-75.
CALDECOTT, J. O. 1986. An ecological and behavioral study of the pig-tailed macaque. Contrib. Primatol., Vol.21. Karger, Basel.
CHEVALIER-SKOLNIKOFF, S., 1974. The ontogeny of communication in stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Contrib. Primatol., Vol.2., Karger, Basel.
CHENG, J. N. 1989. Sexual behaviour and parental care of Formosan macaque (Macaca cyclopis) in Taipei City Zoo. Master thesis, National Taiwan Univ., Taipei (in Chinese)
CHI, C. J. 1989. Agonistic behavior of Formosan macaque (Macaca cyclopis) in Taipei City zoo. Master thesis, National Taiwan Univ., Taipei (in Chinese)
CRONIE, J. E.; CANN, R.; SARICH, V. M. 1980. Molecular evolution and systematic of the genus Macaca. In: The Macaques: Studies in Ecology, Behavior and Evolution, LINDBURG, D. G. (ed.), Van Nostrand Renhold Co., New York, pp. 31-51.
DEPUTTE, F. L.; GOUSTARD, M. 1980. Copulatory vocalizations of female macaques (Macaca fascicularis) variability factor analysis. Primates, 21: 83-99.
DIEN, Z. M. 1985. The Formosan rock-monkey. Quart. J. Taiwan Mus., 11: 345-348.
DITTUS, W. P. G. 1984. Toque monkey food calls: semantic communi- cation concern food distribution in the environment. Anim. Behav., 32: 470-477.
DITTUS, W. P. G. 1988. An analysis of toque macaque cohesion calls from an ecological perspective. In: Primate vocal communication TODT, D.; GOEDEKING, P.; SYMMES, D. (eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp.31-50.
EUDEY, A. A. 1979. Differentiation and dispersal of macaques in Asia. Dissertation, Univ. of California, Davis.
EUDEY, A. A. 1980. Pleistocene glacial phenomena and the evolution of Asian macaques. In: The Macaques: Studies in Ecology, Behavior and Evolution, LINDBURG, D. G. (ed.), Van Nostrand Renhold Co., New York, pp. 52-83.
FISHER, J.; HAMMERSCHMIDT, K.; TODT, D. 1995. Local variation in Barbary macaque shrill barks. Anim. Behav., 56: 623-629.
FISHER, J.; HAMMERSCHMIDT, K.; TODT, D. 1998. Factors affecting acoustic variation in Barbary-macaque (Macaca sylvanus) disturbance calls. Ethology, 101: 51-66.
FOODEN, J. 1980. Classification and distribution of living macaques (Macaca lacepede 1799). In: The Macaques: Studies in Ecology, Behavior and Evolution. LINBURGH, D. G. (ed.), Van Nostrand Rehold Co., New York, pp.1-10.
GAUTIER, J. P. 1971. Etude morphologique et fonctionnelle des annexes extra-layngées des Cercopithcinae; liaison avec les cris de’espacement. Biol. Gabon, 7: 229-267.
GAUTIER, J. P.; GAUTIER, A. 1977. Communication in old world monkeys. In: How Animal Communication. SEBEOK, T. A. (ed.), Indiana Univ. Press, Bloomington, pp.890-964.
GREEN, S. 1975. Variation in vocal pattern with social situation in the Japanese monkey(Macaca fuscata): a field study. In: Primate Behavior: Developments in Field and Laboratory Research, ROSENBLUM, A. (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 1-102.
GREEN, S. 1981. Sex differences and age graduations in vocalizations of Japanese and liontailed monkeys (Macaca fuscata and Macaca silenus). Am. Zool., 21: 165-183.
GREEN, S.; MARLER, P. 1979. The analysis of animal communication. In: Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology Social Behavior and Communication, MARLER, P.; VANDENBERGH, J. G. (eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 73-147.
GRIMM, R. J. 1967. Catalogue of sounds of the pigtailed macaque (Macaca nemestrina). Zoologist, Lond., 152: 361-373.
GROVE, C. P. 1980. Speciation in Macaca: the view from Sulawesi. In: The Macaques: Studies in Ecology, Behavior and Evolution, LINDBURG, D. G. (ed.), Van Nostrand Renhold Co., New York, pp. 84-124.
GOUZOULES, S.; GOUZOULES, H.; MARLER, P. 1984. Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) sreams: representational signaling in recruitment of agonistic aid. Anim. Behav., 32: 182-193.
GOUZOULES, H.; GOUZOULES, S. 1990. Matrilineal signatures in the recruitment screams of pigtail macaques, Macaca nemestrina. Behaviour, 115: 327-347.
GOUZOULES, H.; GOUZOULES, S.; TOMASZYCKI, M. 1998. Agonistic screams and the classification of dominance relationships: are monkeys fuzzy logicians? Anim. Behav., 55: 51-60.
HAAS, A. 1979. The acquisition of genderflect. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 327: 101-113.
HAMMERSCHMIDT, K.; FISHER, J. 1998. The vocal repertoire of Barbary macaques: a quantitative analysis of a graded signal system. Ethology, 104: 203-216.
HAMILTON, W. T.; ARROWOOD, P. C., 1978. Copulatory vocalizations of chacma baboons (Papio ursinus), gibbons (Hylobates hoolock), and humans. Science, 200: 1405-1409.
HAUSER, M. D. 1991. Sources of acoustic variation in rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) vocalizations. Ethology, 89: 29-46.
HAUSER, M. D. 1993. The evolution of nonhuman primate vocalizations: effects of phylogeny, body weight, and social context American Naturalist, 142: 528-542.
HAUSER, M. D.; MARLER, P. 1993a. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). I. Socioecological factors influencing call production. Behavioral Ecology, 4: 194-205.
HAUSER, M. D.; MARLER, P. 1993b. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behavioral Ecology, 4: 206-212.
HERZOG, M. O.; HOHMANN, G. M. 1984. Male Loud calls in Macaca silenus and Presbytis johnii: A comparison. Folia primatol., 43: 189-197.
HOELZER, G. A.; MELNICH, D. J. 1996. Evolutionary relationship of the macaques. In: Evolution and ecology of the macaque societies, FA, J. E.; LINDBURG, D. G. (eds.), Cambridge Univ. Press, pp. 3-19.
HOHMANN, G. M. 1989. Vocal communication of wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Primates, 30: 325-345.
HOHMANN, G. M. 1991. Comparative analyses of age- and sex-specific patterns of vocal behaviour in four species of old world monkeys. Folia Primatol., 56: 133-156.
HOHMANN, G. M.; HERZOG, M. O. 1985. Vocal communication in lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). Folia Primatol., 45: 148-178.
HSU, M. J.; LIN, J. F. 2001. Troop size and Structure in Free-range Formosan macaques (Macaca cyclopis) at Mt. Longevity, Taiwan. Zoological Studies, 40: 49-60.
HSU, M. J.; AGORAMOORTHY, G.; LIN, J. F. 2001.Birth seasonality and interbirth intervals in free-ranging Formosan macaques, Macaca cyclopis, at Mt. Longevity, Taiwan. Primates, 42: 15-25.
ITANI, J. 1963. Vocal communication in the wild Japanese monkey. Primates, 4: 11-66.
KAWAI, M.; MITO, U. 1973. Quantitative study of activity patterns and postures of Formosan Monkeys by the radiotelemetrical technique. Primates, 14: 179-194.
KAWAMURA, S.; NORIKOSHI, K.; AZUMA, N. 1991. Observation of Formosan monkeys (Macaca cyclopis) in Taipingshan, Taiwan. In: Primatology Today, EHARA, A.; KIMURA, T.; TAKENAKA, O.; IWAMOTO, M. (eds.), Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 97-100.
KUMMER, H. 1967. Tripartite relations in hamadryads baboons. In: Social Communication Among Primates, ALTMANN, S. A. (ed.), Chicago Univ. Press, Chicago & London, pp. 63-71.
LEE, L. L.; LIN, Y. S. 1995. Status of Formosan macaques in Taiwan. Primate Consev., 11: 18-20.
LILLEHEI, R. A.; SNOWDON, C. T. 1978. Individual and situational differences in the vocalization of young stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Behaviour, 65: 270-281.
LIN, C. D.; WANG, J. P. 1991. A preliminary survey of Formosan macaque population and habitat utilization in Kaohsiung Chaishan. Taipei: Council of Agriculture, pp.50. (in Chinese)
LINDBURG, D. C. 1971. The rhesus monkey of North India: an ecological and behavioral study. In: Primate Behavior, ROSENBLUM, A. (ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 2-106.
LU, J. F.; LIN, Y. S.; LEE, L. L. 1991. Troop composition, activity pattern and habitat utilization of Formosan macaque (Macaca cyclopis) at Nanshi logging road in Yushan National Park. In: Primatology Today, Proceedings of the XIIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society, EHARA, A.; KIMURA, T.; TAKENAKA, O.; IWAMOTO, M. (eds.), Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 93-96.
MANSON, J. H. (1996). Rhesus macaque copulation calls: re-evaluating the “honest signal” hypothesis. Primates 37: 145-154.
MARLER, P. 1973. A comparison of vocalizations of red-tailed monkeys and blue monkeys, Cercopithecus ascanius and Cercopithecus mitis, in Uganda. Z. Tierpsychol, 33: 223-247.
MARLER, P. 1976. Social organization, communication and graded signals: the chimpanzees and the gorilla. In: Growing Points in Ethology, BATESON, P. P.; HINDE, R. A. (eds.), Cambridge Univ. Press, London, pp. 239-279.
MARLER, P. 1977. The structure of animal communication sounds. In: Recognition of Complex Acoustic Signals, BULLOCK, T. H. (ed.), Springer Verlag, Berlin, pp. 17-35.
MASATAKA, N. 1983. Categorical responses to natural and synthesized alarm calls in Goeldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Primates, 24: 40-51.
MASUI, K.; NARITA, V.; TANAKA, S. 1986. Information on the distribution of Formosan monkeys (Macaca cyclopis). Primates, 27: 383-392.
MELNIK, D. T.; KIDD, K. K. 1985. Genetic and evolutionary relationships among Asian macaques. Int. J. Primatol., 6: 123-160.
MITANI, J. C. 1985. Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour, 92: 59-96.
MOODY, D. B.; STEBBINS, W. C.; MAY, B. J. 1990. Auditory perception of communication signals by Japanese monkeys. In: Comparative Perception: Complex Perception, vol. 2, Complex Signals, STEBBINS, W. C.; BERKLEY, M. A. (eds.), John Wiley and sons, New York, pp.311-344.
MORTON, E. S. 1975. Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. American Naturalist, 109: 17-34.
MORTON, E. S. 1977.On the occurrence and significance of motivation- structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist, 111: 855-869.
MUROYAMA, Y.; THIERRY, B. 1998. Species differences of male loud calls and their perception in Sulawesi macaques. Primates, 39: 115-126.
NISHIMURA, A. 1973. Age changes of the vocalization in free-ranging Japanes monkeys. Symp. Fourth Int. Congr. Primt., Vol. 1, Precultural primate behavior, pp. 76-87. Karger, Basel.
PALOMBIT, R. A. 1992. A preliminary study of vocal communication in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascisularis). I. Vocal repertoire and call emission. Int. J. Primatol., 13: 143-182.
POIRIER, F. E.; DAVIDSON, C. M. 1979. A preliminary study of the Taiwan macaque. Quert. J. Taiwan Mus., 32: 123-191.
ROWELL, T. A.; HINDE, R. A. 1962. Vocal communication by rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Proc. Zool. Soc., Lond. 128: 279-294.
SNOWDON, C. T.; POLA, Y. V. 1978. Interspecific and intraspecific response to synthesized pygmy marmoset vocalization. Anim. Behav., 26: 192-206.
STRUHSAKER, T. T. 1967. Auditory communication among vervet mon- keys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Social communication among Primates, ALTMANN, S. (ed.), Chicago Univ. Press, Chicago and London, pp. 281-324.
STRUHSAKER, T. T.; LELAND, L. 1979. Socioecology of five sympatric monkey species in the Kibale forest, Uganda. In: Advances in the study of behavior. ROSENBLATT, A.; HINDE, R. A.; BEER; BUSNEL (eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp.159-228.
TOKURA, K. 1962. A study on the sexual behavior in the Japanese monkey troop. Primates, 3:1-40.
VAN SCHAIK, C. P. & VAN NOORDWIJK, M. A. 1985. Evolutionary effects of the absentce of felids on the social organization of the macaques of the island of Simeulue (Macaca fascicularis fusca, MILLER, 1903). Folia Primatol., 44: 138-147.
VOGEL, C. 1973. Acoustical communication among free-ranging common Indian langurs (Presbytis entellus) in two different habitats of North India. Am. J. Phys. Anthrop., 38: 469-480.
WASER, P. 1977. Individual recognition, intragroup cohesion, and intergroup spacing: evidence from sound playback to forest monkeys. Behaviour, 60: 28-74.
WILEY, R. H.; RICHARDS, D. G. 1978. Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 3: 69-94.
WHEATLEY, B. P., 1999. The vocal repertoire of Macaca fascicularis at the monkey forest at Padangtegal. In: The Sacred Monkeys of Bali, Waveland Press, USA, pp.154-163.
WU, H. Y.; LIN, Y. S. 1993. Life history variables of wild troops of Formosan Macaques (Macaca cyclopis) in Kenting, Taiwan. Primates, 33: 85-97.

電子全文 Fulltext
本電子全文僅授權使用者為學術研究之目的,進行個人非營利性質之檢索、閱讀、列印。請遵守中華民國著作權法之相關規定,切勿任意重製、散佈、改作、轉貼、播送,以免觸法。
論文使用權限 Thesis access permission:校內校外完全公開 unrestricted
開放時間 Available:
校內 Campus: 已公開 available
校外 Off-campus: 已公開 available


紙本論文 Printed copies
紙本論文的公開資訊在102學年度以後相對較為完整。如果需要查詢101學年度以前的紙本論文公開資訊,請聯繫圖資處紙本論文服務櫃台。如有不便之處敬請見諒。
開放時間 available 已公開 available

QR Code